Result card
|
Authors: Lotte Groth Jensen, Claus Loevschall, Anne Lee
Internal reviewers: Felix Gurtner, Allessandra Lo Scalzo
25 studies were included, 21 including QoL and using different scales (included in the table below) and four studies using a qualitative methodology (not included in the table).
Being offered the chance to participate in a screening programme may trigger both negative and positive reactions among those invited. Experiences from other screening programmes indicate that different psychological effects may occur when implementing a screening programme. These include fear, anxiety and other psychometric reactions. In an ideal research design of a screening programme, you would have to consider the effects of being invited to participate in the screening programme and the effects of accepting the invitation. The next step would be to examine whether participants react different accordingly to the result of the screening test. The most informative design would be to compare participants from the screening programme with a matched population, not participating in the screening programme, instead of comparing subgroups within the screening programme.
Except from the qualitative studies, all the studies included in this review measure psychological changes by means of QoL measurement.
In an overall perspective, a screening programme for AAA contains different points in time, where it seems relevant to measure QoL. Figure 2 illustrates these points in time.
Figure 2: An outline of relevant points in time for measuring quality of life among people invited to participate in a screening programme for AAA
Table 1 displays all the quantitative studies included in the review in terms of e.g. design, study quality and effects. This provides an overview of the effects on QoL, following screening for AAA. The participants are classified in three groups. Overall there are no significant differences between the groups, but a description and interpretation of the results follows below the table.
Table 1: Comparison of Quality of Life in different patients groups, following screening for AAA
Study, location and date |
Design/ Follow-up |
Outcome measure |
Quality (study) |
Effects – cross-sectional or follow-up <= 12 months |
Effects follow-up > 12 months | ||||
Better |
No diff. |
Worse |
Better |
No diff. |
Worse | ||||
Patients with small AAA/or on waiting list (AAA) for operation compared with screened negative patients or non-screened controls | |||||||||
Khaira et al. {4}, UK, 1998 |
Cross-sectional |
HADS |
Low |
+ | |||||
Wanhainen et al. {5} Sweden, 2004 |
Cohort study, 12 months |
SF-36 |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Ashton et al. {6}, UK, 2002, i) |
RCT, 6 weeks |
HADS, SF-36, EQ-5D |
High |
+ |
+ | ||||
Lindholt et al. {7}, Denmark, 2000, i) |
Cross-sectional |
ScreenQL |
Low |
+ | |||||
Lucarotti et al. {8}, UK, 1997 |
Cohort study, 1 month |
GHQ |
Low |
+ | |||||
Spencer et al. {9}, Australia, 2004, i) |
Cohort study, 12 months |
One compo-nent from SF-36 |
Low |
+ | |||||
Spencer et al. {9}, Australia, 2004, i) |
Cross-sectional |
HADS, SF-36, EQ-5D |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm compared with an elective group or matched population | |||||||||
Korhonen et al. {10}, Finland, 2003 |
Cross-sectional |
RAND-36 |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Hennesy et al. {11}, Ireland, 1998 |
Cross-sectional |
HSCL, GHQ |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Hinterseher et al. {12}, Germany, 2004 |
Cross-sectional |
WHO-QOL-BREF-test |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Joseph et al. {13}, USA, 2002 |
Cross-sectional |
SF-36 |
Low |
+ |
+ | ||||
Laukontaus et al. {14}, Finland, 2003 |
Cross-sectional |
EQ-5D |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Hill et al. {15}, Canada, 2007 |
Cross-sectional |
SF-36 |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Tambyraja et al. {16}, Scotland, 2005, iv) |
Cross-sectional |
SF-36 |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Eksandari et al. {17}, USA, 1998, vi) |
Cross-sectional |
SF-36 (telephone interview) |
Low |
+ | |||||
Tambyraja et al. {18}, Scotland, 2004 |
Review |
Primarily |
Moderate |
+ | |||||
Patients operated for small AAA/AAA compared with surveillance and/or controls | |||||||||
Ashton et al. {6}, UK, 2002, i) |
RCT, 3 and 12 months |
HADS, SF-36, EQ-5D |
High |
+ | |||||
Kurz et al. {19}, Switzerland, 2010, ii) |
Cross-sectional |
NHP |
Low |
+ | |||||
De Rango et al. {20}, Italy, 2010, iii) |
RCT, 6 and > 12 months |
SF-36 |
High |
+ |
+ | ||||
Lindholt et al. {7}, Denmark, 2000, i) and iii) |
Cross-sectional |
ScreenQL |
Low |
+ | |||||
Sandström et al. {21}, Sweden, 1996, v) |
Cross-sectional |
SIP, HI |
Moderate |
+ |
+ | ||||
Lederle et al. {22}, USA, 2003, iii) |
RCT, up to 8 years |
SF-36 |
Moderate |
+ |
+ | ||||
Forbes et al. {23}UK, 1998, iii) |
RCT, 12 months |
MOS Short-Form general health survey |
Moderate |
+ |
+ |
i) Studies using more than one study design or different follow-up, ii) Octogenarians were compared with a younger group of patients in a crossover design, iii) Patients operated for small aneurysms, iv) Operative vs. elective, v) Operative vs. non-operative, vi) Operative vs. population
Patients with small AAA/or on waiting list (AAA) for operation compared with patients whose screening results were negative or non-screened controls
Some of the studies indicate that patients diagnosed with at small AAA are more affected, in terms of QoL, than other groups identified in relation to screening for AAA. In Lindholt 2000, patients diagnosed with a small AAA, had the lowest QoL compared with a group of age and gender matched people, who did not attend the screening programme. Patients diagnosed with a small AAA were enrolled in a control set up with scanning of the aorta at fixed intervals. While participating in this control set up, the patients got worse in terms of QoL. However the differences between Lindholt 2000 and the other studies are considered minor {9}.
A qualitative study by Petterson et al. showed that some of the patients diagnosed with a small AAA felt that the frequent follow-up gave rise to questions about what would happen if it ruptured and to thoughts about death. Waiting for surgery was for some people experienced as similar to waiting for a death sentence. On the other hand almost all patients expressed gratitude that the AAA had been discovered. Gratitude for being alive appears to have outweighed the suffering and the sacrifice of well-being following treatment {24}. Letterstål et al. also reported, in a qualitative study, that understanding the seriousness of the situation created distress while waiting for surgery. Sleep disturbances caused by nightmares and thoughts were also experienced by some of the patients {2}. It is possible that the qualitative studies capture some of the psychological effects of screening for AAA, which are not captured in the generic measurements of QoL.
Ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm compared with an elective group or matched population
In nine studies QoL was assessed in patients operated on for a ruptured AAA. Eight studies used a cross-sectional design {10-17} and one study is a review {18}. The studies are mainly of a moderate quality regarding internal validity. QoL is measured using six different validated QoL scales. Overall no difference in QoL could be shown between the groups. As described in the table, QoL in patients operated for a ruptured AAA was compared with QoL in an elective group of patients operated for AAA or QoL in a matched population. Five cross-sectional studies compared patients with a ruptured AAA to a normal population, and three cross-sectional studies to elective patients. Typically the studies identified patients from historical patient records and then measured QoL (self-rated) in a cross-sectional design. Laukontaus et al. showed a significantly lower EQ-5D score among survivors, than in the background population {14}. This result is inconsistent with Korhonen et al. (who used the same basis for recruitment) {10}. The difference in results may be explained by different questionnaires and a small sample size.
There is a risk of selection bias in the studies since a considerable number of the patients operated for a ruptured AAA died, before the researchers could collect relevant data. Furthermore it is also possible that healthier patients are more likely to answer questionnaires than less healthy patients.
In spite of the weak designs of the studies and the risk of bias, the results taken as a whole do not imply any differences in QoL in patients operated for a ruptured AAA compared with a group of elective patients or an age- and sex-adjusted normal population. Consequently most studies suggest that survivors of ruptured AAA can expect a good QoL, which is comparable to that of patients undergoing elective repair or a normal population.
Patients operated for small AAA or AAA compared with surveillance and/or controls
In seven studies QoL was assessed in patients operated on for a small AAA or AAA compared with patients undergoing surveillance or healthy controls. Three studies used a cross-sectional design {7,19,21} and four studies were randomised controlled trials (RCTs) {6,20,22,23}. The studies vary in quality regarding internal validity from low to high with two RCTs being of high quality. Eight different fully (or partially) validated and one non-validated (ScreenQL) QoL scales were used to measure QoL, with follow-up times from 3 months to 8 years in the RCTs.
In general no difference in QoL can be shown between the groups. There is a tendency towards better, short lasting effect on QoL in patients undergoing surgery. However, the effect levels out in time.
Four studies investigate patients with a small AAA, and two studies examine patients with AAA > 5.5 cm. Sandström et al. examined patients with AAA between 4.7 and 7.5 cm {21}. De Rango et al. showed changes in mean SF-36 scores at 6 months from baseline that were significantly higher for early EVAR patients than for surveillance patients, although the effect size is considered small {20}. The effect levelled out in the final follow-up. In spite of the varying study designs and varying quality of the studies, the results above all point in the direction of no difference in QoL in patients operated for an AAA compared with patients under surveillance or controls. Some short-term effects can be identified, but it does not change the overall picture, which suggests that there is no difference between the groups.
Two qualitative studies, one by Langenberg et. al. {25} and one by Brannstorm et al.{26}, show that the participants use different coping strategies when confronted with diagnoses of AAA and that patients generally derive reassurance from the professionals and the professional set-up around the screening programme.
All things considered, this review does not reveal any significant changes or differences in quality of life following screening for AAA. If screening for AAA is implemented, it might be advisable to pay close attention to the group of patients diagnosed with a small AAA and participating in a control set up. Some of the studies indicate that these patients might constitute a particularly vulnerable group.
A weakness in this review is the fact that a lot of the studies included lack a control group outside the study. The studies are designed to compare groups within the screening programme and not with an outside control group.
2. Letterstal A, Eldh AC, Olofsson P, Forsberg C. Patients experience of open repair of abdominal aortic aneurysm--Preoperative information, hospital care and recovery. Journal of Clinical Nursing. 2010;19(21-22):3112-22.
4. Khaira HS, Herbert LM, Crowson MC. Screening for abdominal aortic aneurysms does not increase psychological morbidity. Ann RColl Surg Engl. 1998;80(5):341-2.
5. Wanhainen A, Rosen C, Rutegard J, Bergqvist D, Bjorck M. Low quality of life prior to screening for abdominal aortic aneurysm: a possible risk factor for negative mental effects. Ann Vasc Surg. 2004;18(3):287-93.
6. Ashton HA, Buxton MJ, Day NE, Kim LG, Marteau TM, Scott RA, et al. The Multicentre Aneurysm Screening Study (MASS) into the effect of abdominal aortic aneurysm screening on mortality in men: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2002;360(9345):1531-9.
7. Lindholt JS, Vammen S, Fasting H, Henneberg EW. Psychological consequences of screening for abdominal aortic aneurysm and conservative treatment of small abdominal aortic aneurysms. EurJ Vasc Endovasc Surg. 2000;20(1):79-83.
8. Lucarotti ME, Heather BP, Shaw E, Poskitt KR. Psychological morbidity associated with abdominal aortic aneurysm screening. EurJ Vasc Endovasc Surg. 1997;14(6):499-501.
9. Spencer CA, Norman PE, Jamrozik K, Tuohy R, Lawrence-Brown M. Is screening for abdominal aortic aneurysm bad for your health and well-being? ANZ J Surg. 2004;74(12):1069-75.
10. Korhonen SJ, Kantonen I, Pettila V, Keranen J, Salo JA, Lepantalo M. Long-term survival and health-related quality of life of patients with ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm. EurJ Vasc Endovasc Surg. 2003;25(4):350-3.
11. Hennessy A, Barry MC, McGee H, O'Boyle C, Hayes DB, Grace PA. Quality of life following repair of ruptured and elective abdominal aortic aneurysms. EurJ Surg. 1998;164(9):673-7.
12. Hinterseher I, Saeger HD, Koch R, Bloomenthal A, Ockert D, Bergert H. Quality of life and long-term results after ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm. EurJ Vasc Endovasc Surg. 2004;28(3):262-9.
13. Joseph AY, Fisher JB, Toedter LJ, Balshi JD, Granson MA, Meir-Levi D. Ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm and quality of life. Vasc Endovascular Surg. 2002;36(1):65-70.
14. Laukontaus SJ, Pettila V, Kantonen I, Salo JA, Ohinmaa A, Lepantalo M. Utility of surgery for ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm. Ann Vasc Surg. 2006;20(1):42-8.
15. Hill AB, Palerme LP, Brandys T, Lewis R, Steinmetz OK. Health-related quality of life in survivors of open ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm repair: a matched, controlled cohort study. J Vasc Surg. 2007;46(2):223-9.
16. Tambyraja AL, Fraser SC, Murie JA, Chalmers RT. Functional outcome after open repair of ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm. J Vasc Surg. 2005;41(5):758-61.
17. Eskandari MK, Bowle SA, Webster MW, Steed DL, Makaroun MS, Muluk SC, et al. Ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysms in the 1990s: Resource utilization, long-term survival, and quality of life after repair. Vascular Surgery. 1998;32(5):415-424.
18. Tambyraja AL, Fraser SC, Murie JA, Chalmers RT. Quality of life after repair of ruptured abdominal aortic aneurysm. European Journal of Vascular and Endovascular Surgery. 2004;28:229-33.
19. Kurz M, Meier T, Pfammatter T, mann-Vesti BR. Quality of life survey after endovascular abdominal aortic aneurysm repair in octogenarians. Int Angiol. 2010;29(3):249-54.
20. De Rango P, Verzini F, Parlani G, Cieri E, Romano L, Loschi D, et al. Quality of life in patients with small abdominal aortic aneurysm: the effect of early endovascular repair versus surveillance in the CAESAR trial. EurJ Vasc Endovasc Surg. 2011;41(3):324-31.
21. Sandstrom V, Bjorvell H, Olofsson P. Functional status and well-being in a group of patients with abdominal aortic aneurysm. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences. 1996;10(3):186-91.
22. Lederle FA, Johnson GR, Wilson SE, Acher CW, Ballard DJ, Littooy FN, et al. Quality of life, impotence, and activity level in a randomized trial of immediate repair versus surveillance of small abdominal aortic aneurysm. J Vasc Surg. 2003;38(4):745-52.
23. Health service costs and quality of life for early elective surgery or ultrasonographic surveillance for small abdominal aortic aneurysms. UK Small Aneurysm Trial Participants. Lancet. 1998;352(21):1656-60.
24. Pettersson M, Bergbom I. The drama of being diagnosed with an aortic aneurysm and undergoing surgery for two different procedures: open repair and endovascular techniques. J Vasc Nurs. 2010;28(1):2-10.
25. Langenberg D, Abholz HH. How do patients cope with controllable abdominal aneurysm?. [German]. Zeitschrift fur Allgemeinmedizin. 2003;79:32-35.
26. Brannstrom M, Bjorck M, Strandberg G, Wanhainen A. Patients' experiences of being informed about having an abdominal aortic aneurysm - a follow-up case study five years after screening. J Vasc Nurs. 2009;27(3):70-4.